Do face masks halve the risk of Covid? A note on the evidence
By Professor Simon Wood
«Lost of the trouble in life comes from misunderstanding’ as Willy Loman said in Oscar Wilde’s masterpiece An Inspector Calls. Grating isn’t it? But also a reasonable facsimile of how science reporting often reads, at least to those of us self identifying as scientists. Pre-Covid it was so. With Covid the problem has become, well, an epidemic. Recent reporting about the efficacy of masks is a classic case in point.
You might remember the study in question: it certainly made headlines when it came out. 'Mask-wearing cuts Covid incidence by 53%, says global study', stated the Guardian: 'results from more than 30 studies from around the world were analysed in detail'. The New Scientist agreed: 'Covid-19 news: Mask wearing cuts infections by 53 per cent', but instead referenced a greater number of evidence bases: 'using data from 72 studies'. A more cautious Times reported the same results as 'Masks more effective than distancing and handwashing'. It goes for 'more than 30 studies from around the world' which 'were analysed in detail'. Bloomberg claimed 'Masks Cut Covid Risk in Half', now based on a far-lower number: eight studies.
Let’s leave aside the nitpicking details (infections and risk are not the same thing, for example) — surely these reports are usefully communicating the gist of the new evidence, are they not? I’m afraid not.
The BMJ paper at the centre of these reports is a meta-analysis. That is to say: a summary, an attempt to combine the evidence contained in several scientific studies to arrive at an objective assessment of the state of knowledge on a topic.
The analysis identified 72 studies that might potentially have provided evidence on the effectiveness of masks, social distancing and hand washing. Of those, just six (not eight, 30 or 72) were sufficiently relevant — and of sufficient quality — that they could provide any useful information on mask efficacy. And how reliable were the six? Four were assessed to have a moderate risk of bias, and two to have a serious or critical risk.
Let’s start with the moderate four.
Moving on we have Wang et al. (2020), who looked at transmission only within the same household. If at least one household member was always wearing a mask at home before anyone in the house got sick, then there was a reduced risk of transmission. So the ultra-cautious are at lower risk of something irrelevant to the discussion of public mask-wearing.
Xu et al. (2020) was based on an online survey, but I could not relate the results in the paper to those given in Talic et al.
Fourthly we have Bundgaard (2020), the famously inconclusive ‘Danmask’ study of 6,024 Danish citizens and the only one of the six based on the sort of randomised trial that usually counts as evidence for a medical intervention.
Finally, we come to the studies deemed to be at risk of serious or critical bias:
Krishnamachari et al. (2021) compare the timing of mask mandates in different areas of the US and contrast subsequent case rates. It is unclear how such a study provides a measure of relative transmission risk that can be combined with the other five.
Doung-ngern et al. (2020), on the other hand, did attempt to look at the actual risk of transmission: to the contacts of people who subsequently turned out to be infected. If the contact wore a mask throughout the interaction then they were at reduced risk of picking up the infection. This might seem compelling until you think about the sort of interactions during which you do not remove your mask, as compared to those in which you do.
You should form your own judgement of how much evidence all this can provide for the efficacy of public mask-wearing as a Covid mitigation strategy, but it is certainly not as clear-cut as generally reported. It’s also interesting that the fact-checkers, who were so quick to crack down on reports suggesting low mask efficacy, especially the Danmask study, seem to have been less worried this time around.
My view is that the misunderstanding and unbalanced reporting of science leads to a great deal of trouble. Masks are the least of it (it’s Anne of the Island, L.M. Montgomery, by the way).»
Simon Wood is a professor of statistics at the University of Edinburgh.
Vós não fazeis ideia de como este tema é maligno.
ResponderEliminarHá estudos correctos desde o início da década de 1980 até 2013 que mostram não haver diferenças entre o número de infecções pós-operatórias e o uso de toucas ou de máscaras pelas equipas cirúrgicas. Dados que me assarapantaram mas estão bem documentados.
As máscaras assinaladas na propaganda de todos os governos têm poros maiores umas 20 a 40 vezes do que as dimensões de qualquer vírus.
Será como as redes usadas nas capoeiras: evitam a passagem de galinhas e pombos mas deixam passar varejeiras e moscas vulgares.
Abraço